The rules of football stipulate that if a
foul is committed inside a defending player’s own penalty area, a penalty kick
is awarded to the attacking team. A high proportion of penalty kicks are
converted to goals so penalties can be decisive, particularly in low scoring
games. It is therefore especially important for referees’ decisions about
penalty kicks to be accurate. However recent evidence [1] shows that these decisions
are subject to subtle bias effects. A study of a large number of penalty
decisions in the German Bundesliga concluded that referees tend to ‘even out’
penalty decisions between opposing teams.
The study analysed data from 12902 Bundesliga
matches over 40 years, in which 3723 penalties were awarded. The results showed
that more matches than would be expected on statistical grounds involved
two penalties. The second penalty in the match tended to be awarded to the team
that was not awarded the first penalty, indicative of a ‘compensation bias’, a
tendency for referees to balance out their decisions. A reasonable alternative
explanation for the excess of second penalties is that after a successful
penalty conversion the offending team goes on the attack to rectify the score,
and is therefore more likely to be awarded the second penalty. But the study
found that the bias to award the second penalty to the offending team only
occurred when the first penalty was successfully converted rather than missed.
What could cause such a bias? Research on human
perception has revealed that sensory information is generally much more
ambiguous than we appreciate, so decisions about what we ‘see’ are more
difficult than they might appear. We unconsciously resolve ambiguity by
putting together incoming sensory data with stored knowledge in order to reach a decision about
what we see [2].
Decisions made by sports officials are
subject to the same limitations as other decisions in perception, so they often
have to be made on the basis of partial or ambiguous sensory data. For instance,
a football referee may have to decide whether a particular offence occurred just
inside the penalty area or just outside it. For very close calls of this kind
the visual evidence can be quite ambiguous (see [3] for an analysis of tennis line calls) so even the best referees are liable to make mistakes on
occasion.
The problems of ambiguity and uncertainty
are not confined to decisions about the location of a foul. It can be unclear, for example, whether the tackling defender made illegal bodily
contact with the attacker. Attacking players are very skilful at manufacturing
visual evidence consistent with contact (diving), and defenders are skillful at
concealing evidence of contact (shirt pulls).
So penalty decisions depend on a complex
process of combining uncertain sensory evidence with decision-making factors
such as previous experience, rule interpretation and knowledge of the teams and
players. The referee must maintain a criterion for deciding whether the
evidence is sufficient to award a penalty. The criterion may well shift
unconsciously during a game on the basis of, for example, which players are
involved. The compensation bias found in the Bundesliga study may reflect a
shift in the referee’s criterion in favour of a more lenient evidence threshold
for penalties awarded to a previously offending team.
On average one penalty is awarded every 3.48
Bundesliga games. In other words there are about 0.29 penalties per game. How
does this rate of penalty awards compare with major international competitions
involving European teams, namely the World Cup and the Euros?
The recent history of both competitions
shows some interesting trends. In World Cup competitions since 2000 there has
been a decline in penalty awards from 0.28 per game in 2002 (similar to the
Bundesliga) to 0.21 per game in 2014 (a 25% fall). In the Euros over the same
period there has been a much more marked decline from 0.36 per game in 2000 to
0.13 in 2012 (a 66% fall). The large fall in Euro penalties suggests that
referees may be more reluctant to award a penalty than they used to be;
compared to the Bundesliga they award less than half the number of penalties
per game. This shift could reflect the adoption of a much more strict evidence
criterion for the award of a penalty in the Euro competition.
Among the alternative explanations are that Euro games
have became ‘cleaner’, or that players are now more skilled at hiding their
offences, and these changes have led to a decline in penalty awards. However
the number of cautions shown in both the World Cup and the Euros does not support this interpretation: caution rates are in decline in the World
Cup but not in the Euros, and 25% more yellow cards were shown during each game
in the most recent Euro competition (2012) compared to the most recent World
Cup (2014) despite the fact only a third as many penalties were awarded in Euro 2012 [4, 5].
By the end of the Euro 2016 competition, 11 penalties had been awarded in 51 games (not including shoot-outs). This represents an average of one penalty every 4.64 games, or about 0.22 penalties per game, quite similar to recent World Cups but lower than in the Bundesliga. Interestingly, success rate is on the decline and is at the lowest level it has ever been, at 64% of penalties converted. But that is another story.
Reading and sources
1. Schwarz, W. (2011) Compensating tendencies in penalty kick decisions of referees in professional football: Evidence from the German Bundesliga 1963–2006, Journal of Sports Sciences, 29:5, 441-447.
2. Mather, G., Sharman, R.J. (2015) Decision-level adaptation in motion perception. Royal Society Open Science, 2 (12), 150418.
3. Mather G (2008) Perceptual uncertainty and line-call challenges in professional tennis. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B, 275, 1645-1651.
4. http://www.uefa.com/uefaeuro/season=2016/statistics/index.html
5. http://www.fifa.com/worldcup/archive/brazil2014/statistics/index.html
No comments:
Post a Comment